Ir para conteúdo
  • Cadastre-se

Posts Recomendados

Postado

Tenho um tio que (quando mais novo) ele nunca dava bola pra uma menina que gostava dele, pelo fato de ela ser bem mais nova. Hoje em dia estão casados há não sei quantos anos.

 

Esse negócio de idade é complicado no começo, depois dos 25 (dependendo da diferença), já não é relevante.

 

Olha o tanto de pessoas com 50+ namorando gente de 20~30. Alguns casos teve até reportagem.

Acho que eu só namoraria uma de 40 se tivesse rosto de 30 e corpo de 25.

Publicidade

Postado
1 hora atrás, CassioGolden disse:

Galera aconteceu uma merda com um brother meu, ele casou um tempo atras com uma mulher que tem um passado muito sujo, familia ruim e tal, porém essa mina tava frequentando a igreja dele, ele é evangelico e quando falou dessa mina eu aconselhei ele a não se envolver, mas mesmo assim ele se envolveu, casou, teve 1 filho e sempre ouviamos fofoca dela para com ele, ainda mais que ela já tem moto e casa, e ele nao tinha nada, porém é muito trabalhador.

Mulher mal carater mesmo, todos ja sabiam, porém aconteceu algo pior.

Ela recentemente brigou com ele, o atacou a porradas pelo que ele contou, ele disse q nao revidou e ainda disse q estava com o filho no colo, mas mesmo assim ela foi a polícia e alegou que ele tinha batido nela, cortou o proprio cabelo e disse que ele que arrancou, os braços dele estavam todos arranhados por ela, acho que pra simular que houve uma briga pra policia acreditar.

Resultado: A policia pegou ele em casa de surpresa (ela nao tava, a policia chegou com ela) e prendeu ele, ele passou uma noite na cadeira e pra sair pagou fiança de 1.500,00. Seria 5.000,00, porém a sorte que ele tinha gente conhecido dentro da policia. Ele agora foi embora pra outra cidade e está muito abalado, disse que tentaram enforcar ele dentro da cadeia, os presos querendo bater porque ele era acusado de agressao a mulher, deixaram ele sem tomar agua, e entre outras humilhações.


fujam de mulher mal carater galera, tem mulher q nao tem limite.

 

Foi merecido, apesar de ter sido muito injusto.

 

Alguns só aprendem na dor e porrada mesmo...

 

Aí já vem outra questão:

 

A palavra "santa" de qualquer mulher. Mal é feita uma investigação e o cara só por ter um pinto já é automaticamente culpado. O que me deixa triste é que isso não é apenas no Brasil, infelizmente é praticamente no mundo todo. Ser homem em alguns casos é uma merda, e ainda as feminazis acham que é privilégio.

Postado

sobre nofap

 

 

 

quem não conhece o napoleon hill devia conhecer e ler seus trabalhos, o mais famoso deve ser o think and grow rich,

 

o cara foi guru de como enriquecer por décadas e  ainda é...

 

isso é um trecho do livro outwiting the devil

 

 

 

 

 

N: just what damage is there in over-indulgence(excesso) of sex?  (excesso de fap principalmente)

D: the greatest damage is that it depletes the source of man's greatest driving force, and wastes, without adequate compensation, man's creative energy. It dissipates energy needed by nature to maintain physical health. Sex is nature's most useful therapeutic force. It depletes the magnetic energy which is thesource of an attractive pleasing personality. It removes the sparkle from one's eyes and sets up discord in the tone of one's voice. It destroys enthusiasm, subdues ambition, and lead inevitably to the habit of drifting on all sujects. 

N: I would like for you to answer my question in another way by telling me what beneficial ends the emotion of sex may be made to attain, if mastered and transmuted.

D: controlled sex, supplies the magnetic force that attracts people to one another. It is the most important factor of a pleasing personality. It gives quality to the tone of voice and enables one to convey through the voice any feeling desired. It serves, as nothing else can serve, to give motive power to one's desires. It keeps the nervous system charged with the energy needed to carry on the work of maintaining the body. It sharpens the imagination and enables one to create useful ideas. It give quickness and definiteness to one's physical and mental movements. It gives one persistence and perseverance in the pursuit of one's major purpose in life. It is a great antidote for all fear. It gives one immunity against discouragement. Ithelps to master laziness and procrastination. It gives one physical and mental endurance while under going any form of opposition or defeat. It gives one the fighting qualities necessary under all circumstances for self defense. In brief, it makes winners and not quitters!

Postado (editado)
1 hora atrás, CassioGolden disse:

Galera aconteceu uma merda com um brother meu, ele casou um tempo atras com uma mulher que tem um passado muito sujo, familia ruim e tal, porém essa mina tava frequentando a igreja dele, ele é evangelico e quando falou dessa mina eu aconselhei ele a não se envolver, mas mesmo assim ele se envolveu, casou, teve 1 filho e sempre ouviamos fofoca dela para com ele, ainda mais que ela já tem moto e casa, e ele nao tinha nada, porém é muito trabalhador.

Mulher mal carater mesmo, todos ja sabiam, porém aconteceu algo pior.

Ela recentemente brigou com ele, o atacou a porradas pelo que ele contou, ele disse q nao revidou e ainda disse q estava com o filho no colo, mas mesmo assim ela foi a polícia e alegou que ele tinha batido nela, cortou o proprio cabelo e disse que ele que arrancou, os braços dele estavam todos arranhados por ela, acho que pra simular que houve uma briga pra policia acreditar.

Resultado: A policia pegou ele em casa de surpresa (ela nao tava, a policia chegou com ela) e prendeu ele, ele passou uma noite na cadeira e pra sair pagou fiança de 1.500,00. Seria 5.000,00, porém a sorte que ele tinha gente conhecido dentro da policia. Ele agora foi embora pra outra cidade e está muito abalado, disse que tentaram enforcar ele dentro da cadeia, os presos querendo bater porque ele era acusado de agressao a mulher, deixaram ele sem tomar agua, e entre outras humilhações.


fujam de mulher mal carater galera, tem mulher q nao tem limite.

Se fodeu, tudo de ruim pra quem quer ser capitão salva putas e casar com vadia que fingiu se arrepender e virou "religiosa fervorosa", é o script de todas, rodam, rodam, rodam, rodam e quando começam a ficar velhas ou muito mal faladas viram crentes fervorosas e arrumam na igreja algum "varão" otário para assumir e casar com ela e você no outro post perguntando se precisava de prova para ir preso em uma falsa acusação de estupro kkkkkk ser homem é crime, me surpreende ele não ter sido estuprado na cadeia, isso é, se ele não foi e ta com vergonha de contar, quando o cara é enganado e se casa com uma mulher que se fingia de boa moça e depois ele descobre que ela tinha um passado sujo eu até tenho pena, mas otário que casa com rodada não tenho nenhuma, bom que esses retardados servem como espelho para os outros não fazerem a mesma merda no futuro, já que vão ver eles sofrendo falsas acusações, sendo traídos, perdendo tudo e sendo sugado no divórcio após sua dama perceber que cansou de brincar de casinha e que quer voltar a curtir a vida, entre outras coisas.

 

Agora o babaca ainda vai ter que ver ela por muitos anos ou talvez pro resto da vida por causa da criança, sem contar na pensão que ele vai ter que pagar para ela e a criança.

Editado por Cpt Jack
Postado
all 123 comments
exibir primeiro: 
melhores
 

 
 

[–]mikesublime 15 pontos 21 hours ago 

You know, testosterone has provided me with more nootropic benefits than any other substance... with the possible exception of Adderall.

I'm 36/m and I've been on TRT for a little over a year. My levels went from being in the 2-300's to the 8-900's. I take 120mg of testosterone cypionate every week.

There are tons of little benefits, but I'll quickly discuss the two biggest ones. The first benefit is with my social anxiety. I'd tried dozens of different meds (and illegal & non-prescription drugs) over the years (antidepressants, benzos, etc.) and nothing has worked nearly as well as good ol' Vitamin T.

I was expecting a boost in confidence and reduced social anxiety. What I wasn't expecting was testosterone's ability to help me think clearly, logically, and rationally. It's incredible. My ability to problem solve without getting clouded with emotion and irrational thoughts is amazing.

Test has been a game changer for me. Best nootropic ever.

P.S. I know it's not a nootropic.

 

 

 
 

[–]hairnothrowaway 14 pontos 1 dia ago* 

the cognitive effects of T are well known - this is one of the reasons that transdermal anabolic steroids are being marketed over the TV for "low-T"

...anabolic steroids came about in the 1950s - reports on improved cognition and mood have always been associated with use - specifically increased cognition, mood, libido, confidence/potential-aggression

the down-side is that you're tinkering with your endogenous T system, and negative repercussions are well known/documented - for older men 50-80, i believe this "low-T" stuff is just the start to medicinal anabolic steroid use - in other worlds i believe there is huge medicinal potential for T in the elderly population for both psychiatric and physiological conditions

going out on a limb here too (may be wrong) but anabolic steroids + exercise can create large increases in endogenous IGF and other growth hormones essentially causing an endogenous-nootropic cascade

 
 

[–]rrohbeck 14 pontos 23 hours ago 

Corollary: Much of the depression and fog reported these days is caused by lack of exercise.

 
 

[–]ChefLinguini 7 pontos 22 hours ago 

Can confirm. Always feel better when I work out

 
 
 
 
 
 
 

 
 

[–]averageuse 4 pontos 20 hours ago 

Test has made my depression lessen and focus increase to an extent. It builds confidence and makes you more assertive and care less about bullshit. I started blast and cruise about a year ago and it has benefitted me greatly. I have more drive, I work out harder, I'm more level headed and I honestly recommended it to my 2 best friends they are both about to give it a try. The stigma behind steroids causes me to keep it a secret from everyone except 3 close friends, girlfriend has no idea and my family doesn't either. Oh yeah, my anxiety decreased also. These things are all anecdotal and the degrees that it helped me psychologically are not extreme, but I do recommend it to anyone willing to put in the time and effort to use it safely.

Postado

Social status & testosterone

28gwern20 October 2011 02:05PM

We’ve discussed signaling and status endlessly on LW; I think this is right up our vein: a 2011 review of research on the connections between famous male hormone testosterone and various forms of social interaction and especially social status, Eisenegger et al’s “The role of testosterone in social interaction”. (I grabbed this PDF in the short time Elsevier left full-text available, but only now, with some modafinil-powered spare time, have gotten around to excerpting it for you guys.)

1 Abstract

Although animal researchers established the role of testosterone as a ‘social hormone’ decades ago, the investigation of its causal influence on human social behaviors has only recently begun. Here, we review and discuss recent studies showing the causal effects of testosterone on social interactions in animals and humans, and outline the basic neurobiological mechanisms that might underlie these effects. Based on these recent findings, we argue that the role of testosterone in human social behavior might be best understood in terms of the search for, and maintenance of, social status.

2 Excerpts

Is testosterone simply aggression promoting (a sort of ‘roid rage’)?

Early evidence for the role of testosterone in social behavior suggested that it facilitates overt physical aggression (see Glossary) in social contexts. For instance, castrated rodents, which have little, if any, testosterone circulating in their blood, show a near-complete absence of physical fights; however, fights can be fully restored by providing testosterone supplementation to these animals [03]…high testosterone levels in male prisoners have been linked to having a history of rape, murder and armed robbery, and relatively lower levels to a history of theft and drug abuse [08]. A similar pattern was observed in a study of female prison inmates [9]. However, the causality in these studies remains unclear

Probably not:

the existing evidence for a link between aggression and testosterone in humans is relatively weak, but positive [12]. Even if one accepts the fact that reactive aggression can be measured in a controlled laboratory environment, results are similarly inconclusive: recent studies found a positive relationship between baseline testosterone levels and laboratory measures of reactive aggression (reviewed in [11]), but others also reported null findings (in larger samples) [13]. Most importantly, however, a causal role for testosterone in forms of reactive aggression could not be confirmed, as neither long-term nor acute administration of testosterone had an effect [13,14].

This may come as a surprise:

Folk wisdom holds that testosterone causes antisocial, egoistic, or even aggressive behaviors in humans. However, the correlational studies discussed above already suggest that this simple folk view probably requires revision [34,56]. A recent placebo-controlled testosterone administration study found support for the idea that the testosterone-aggression link might be based upon ‘folk’ views: individuals given placebo who believed they had been given testosterone showed less fair bargaining offers compared with those who believed that they had received placebo, thus confirming people’s stereotypes about the behavioral effects of testosterone.

The null findings may be due to a possible confounding effect of homeostasis, but that wouldn’t cover the null on acute administration:

The first study to use a causal testosterone administration procedure in an experimental economic setting did not find any effects on several economic social interactions [14]. Because the study used long-term administration of testosterone, this null finding might be due to secondary feedback effects on the neuroendocrine axis (i.e. suppression of endogenous testosterone production owing to chronic administration). In general, acute administration shows greater reliability in the production of both behavioral and neurophysiological effects (reviewed in [55]).

‘Dominant’ looks like a better perspective than ‘aggressive’:

rhesus monkeys with high testosterone levels use stares, threats and displacements, rather than overtly aggressive interactions, to ascertain high social status [16]…[saliva] measurements of testosterone at a single time-point correlate positively with high dominance in both adolescents [19,20] and adults [21,22]. In addition, salivary testosterone levels correlate with implicit measures of power motivation [23] and increased vigilance for status threats [24,25]. As a result of these relationships, and the moderate stability of testosterone levels over time, some have suggested that baseline testosterone levels reflect a personality trait [26]

men show a larger increase in testosterone when exposed to the scent of an ovulating woman compared with that of a non-ovulating woman or a control [27]. Apart from sexual social stimuli, which are reliable inductors of a testosterone response [28,29], social interactions outside a direct reproductive context have also been shown to induce a testosterone response [01]. In particular, testosterone levels rise within minutes in anticipation of both physical and non-physical competitive situations; for example, dyadic food competition in chimpanzees [30], or tennis, chess or domino tournaments in humans (reviewed in [31]). Testosterone also reacts to contest outcomes [32], and not just to anticipation: for instance, stock traders show higher testosterone levels if their daily profits are above average, and winners of soccer matches show higher testosterone levels than do the losers [33]….causal manipulation of social context (e.g. rigged contests) confirms a causal effect of winning situations on testosterone levels (e.g. [34–38]). These effects can be large; for example, merely watching oneself win a competitive interaction on video produces a 40% testosterone surge from baseline [37].

This interest in dominance leads to mental changes (I am reminded of self-deception):

individuals who generally have higher scores on self-reported dominance and higher basal levels of testosterone show vigilant responses to angry facial expressions (reviewed in [42]). Furthermore, exogenous administration of testosterone increases the sympathetic heart-rate response to angry, but not to happy facial expressions [43] (Figure 2). Although this could theoretically also reflect autonomic arousal as part of a fear response, testosterone has been shown to reduce fear [44], suggesting that dominant people perceive an angry face as a challenge….a recent testosterone administration study has shown that facial mimicry [“a precursor of empathy-related processes occurring automatically”] in response to emotional facial expressions is relatively suppressed after a single dose of testosterone [48]…a single administration of testosterone to young females leads to a significant impairment in the ability to infer emotions, intentions and feelings from the eye region of the face [49]. In addition, the same study established that subjects’ second-to-fourth digit ratio, which is thought to be a marker of prenatal testosterone exposure, is largely able to predict this effect…In line with this are findings of decreased trustworthiness ratings of facial photographs in subjects who received a single dose of testosterone [51]. Crucially, this effect was driven most strongly by those who trusted easily, suggesting that testosterone adaptively increases social vigilance in these trusting individuals to better prepare them for competition over status and valued resources

(The jokes about women and men almost make themselves.)

Not all of these changes are what one would naively expect (see previously about the ‘folk theory’ of testosterone):

one acute dose of testosterone in women increased the fairness of proposers’ bargaining offers in an ultimatum game [13] (Figure 3). An important motive driving proposer behavior is to avoid the rejection of the offer. Thus, if testosterone increases the concern for status, subjects who received testosterone might have perceived a rejection as more aversive, inducing them to make fairer offers…Another study [57] found that testosterone administration prior to an ultimatum game resulted in decreased generosity in a sample of healthy males if repeated measures were not controlled for. The results are insignificant, however, if the fact that the same subject participated in the ultimatum game several time is correctly controlled for statistically. Moreover, a recent study suggests that a low second-to-fourth digit ratio (high prenatal testosterone exposure) is associated with unfair proposer offers if subjects had previously received an unfair offer when in the responder role [58]. Many possible spill-over effects can thus occur in a within-subject design such as that used in [57], where subjects repeatedly play as a proposer and a responder, rendering the interpretation of the results difficult.

I found interesting the material starting page 267, “Neurobiological mechanisms underlying the role of testosterone in social status hierarchies” (due to my own musings about the possible effects of masturbation went that it might be misinterpreted as reproductive ‘success’ which reduces risk-taking or activity in general):

Maintaining a high status position requires an increased sensitivity for aversive events and impending social threats, particularly those that challenge the high social status of an individual. As we show below, testosterone appears to be able to influence such processes; in particular, it appears to confer high motivational drive, low fearfulness and high stress-resilience, either directly or via interactions with other hormones and neurotransmitter systems.

Fear & stress:

Among healthy young men, the blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) response in the amygdala to fearful and angry faces co-varies positively with individual differences in serum testosterone concentrations ([63,64], but see [65]). Exogenous testosterone has been shown to activate the amygdala in young women viewing angry facial expressions [66] (Figure 4). A mechanism underlying these observations might be that testosterone induces a functional decoupling between OFC and amygdala activity [67,68]…In humans, single acute doses of testosterone have been shown to reduce subconscious fear (Figure 5) and fear-potentiated startle [44,89].

…In face-to-face interactions, individuals are assumed to compete for status in fairly well-defined contests, each trying to ‘outstress’ the other with verbal and facial cues, and the fact that low-ranked members show more stress symptoms than higher-ranked members during mutual interaction is a common feature of status hierarchies [40]. Stress probably also plays an important role in anonymous competition. Hence, stress resilience might enable an individual to cope with a challenge adaptively. Studies in animals have confirmed that testosterone downregulates the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal stress response [90]. It has also been shown to attenuate the sympathetically mediated stress response to aversive stimuli in humans [91].

Motivation & learning:

Reward-based reinforcement effects in animals have been observed within short time periods (30 min) after systemic administration of testosterone [79], suggesting that a testosterone surge following a status-relevant social stimulus might reinforces any behavior that led to that testosterone response in the first place. In humans, patients who are hypogonadal (testosterone levels too low) show apathy and lack of motivation [80], whereas testosterone administration in healthy subjects induces motivation to act [81] and upregulates activity in the ventral striatum [82]

Summary of foregoing:

Testosterone administration studies confirm that the hormone also has fear-reducing properties in humans. A further important function of testosterone is its role in motivation; animal models have shown a tight link with the dopaminergic system within striatal areas. Thus, together with the ability to reduce fear and buffer stress responses, testosterone might have a pivotal role in promoting upward movement in a status hierarchy by facilitating the engagement in a competition for status. By contrast, testosterone can promote threat vigilance, which enables an individual to not only detect potential status challenges, but also, as a consequence of, and facilitated through the mechanisms detailed above, act accordingly to defend its high status position. These effects might be mediated by the amygdala

3 References

  • 01: Mazur, A. and Booth, A. (1998) Testosterone and dominance in men. Behav. Brain Sci. 21, 353–363 discussion 363–397
  • 03: Beeman, E.A. (1947) The relation of the interval between castration and 1st encounter to the aggressive behavior of mice. Anat. Rec. 99, 570–571
  • 08: Dabbs, J.M. et al. (1995) Testosterone, crime, and misbehavior among 692 male prison-inmates. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 18, 627–633
  • 11: Carre, J.M. et al. (2011) The social neuroendocrinology of human aggression. Psychoneuroendocrinology DOI: 10.1016/J.PSYNEUEN. 2011.02.001
  • 12: Archer, J. et al. (2005) Testosterone and aggression: a reanalysis of Book, Starzyk, and Quinsey’s (2001) study. Aggress. Violent Behav. 10, 241–261
  • 13: Eisenegger, C. et al. (2010) Prejudice and truth about the effect of testosterone on human bargaining behaviour. Nature 463, 356–359
  • 14: Zethraeus, N. et al. (2009) A randomized trial of the effect of estrogen and testosterone on economic behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 6535–6538
  • 16: Higley, J.D. et al. (1996) CSF testosterone and 5-HIAA correlate with different types of aggressive behaviors. Biol. Psychiatry 40, 1067–1082
  • 19: Vermeersch, H. et al. (2010) Gender ideology, same-sex peer group affiliation and the relationship between testosterone and dominance in adolescent boys and girls. J. Biosoc. Sci. 42, 463–475
  • 20: Rowe, R. et al. (2004) Testosterone, antisocial behavior, and social dominance in boys: pubertal development and biosocial interaction. Biol. Psychiatry 55, 546–552
  • 21: Grant, V.J. and France, J.T. (2001) Dominance and testosterone in women. Biol. Psychol. 58, 41–47
  • 22: Carre, J.M. et al. (2009) Testosterone responses to competition predict future aggressive behaviour at a cost to reward in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 561–570
  • 23: Stanton, S.J. and Schultheiss, O.C. (2009) The hormonal correlates of implicit power motivation. J. Res. Pers. 43, 942
  • 24: van Honk, J. et al. (1999) Correlations among salivary testosterone, mood, and selective attention to threat in humans. Horm. Behav. 36, 17–24
  • 25: Wirth, M.M. and Schultheiss, O.C. (2007) Basal testosterone moderates responses to anger faces in humans. Physiol. Behav. 90, 496–505
  • 26: Sellers, J.G. et al. (2007) Hormones and personality: testosterone as a marker of individual differences. J. Res. Pers. 41, 126–138
  • 27: Miller, S.L. and Maner, J.K. (2010) Scent of a woman: men’s testosterone responses to olfactory ovulation cues. Psychol. Sci. 21, 276–283
  • 28: Roney, J.R. et al. (2007) Rapid endocrine responses of young men to social interactions with young women. Horm. Behav. 52, 326–333
  • 29: Lopez, H.H. et al. (2009) Attractive men induce testosterone and cortisol release in women. Horm. Behav. 56, 84–92
  • 30: Wobber, V. et al. (2010) Differential changes in steroid hormones before competition in bonobos and chimpanzees. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 12457–12462
  • 31: Salvador, A. (2005) Coping with competitive situations in humans. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 195–205
  • 32: Mehta, P.H. and Josephs, R.A. (2006) Testosterone change after losing predicts the decision to compete again. Horm. Behav. 50, 684–692
  • 33: Oliveira, T. et al. (2009) Testosterone responsiveness to winning and losing experiences in female soccer players. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 1056–1064
  • 34: Josephs, R.A. et al. (2006) The mismatch effect: when testosterone and status are at odds. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 90, 999–1013
  • 35: Josephs, R.A. et al. (2003) Status, testosterone, and human intellectual performance: stereotype threat as status concern. Psychol. Sci. 14, 158–163
  • 36: Newman, M.L. et al. (2005) Testosterone, cognition, and social status. Horm. Behav. 47, 205–211
  • 37: Carre, J.M. and Putnam, S.K. (2010) Watching a previous victory produces an increase in testosterone among elite hockey players. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 475–479
  • 38: Schultheiss, O.C. et al. (2005) Effects of implicit power motivation on men’s and women’s implicit learning and testosterone changes after social victory or defeat. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 88, 174–188
  • 40: Mazur, A. (1985) A biosocial model of status in face-to-face primate groups. Soc. Forces 64, 377–402
  • 42: van Honk, J. and Schutter, D.J. (2007) Vigilant and avoidant responses to angry facial expressions. In Social Neuroscience: Integrating Biological and Psychological Explanations of Social Behavior (Harmon-Jones, E. and Winkielman, P., eds), pp. 197–223, The Guilford Press
  • 43: van Honk, J. et al. (2001) A single administration of testosterone induces cardiac accelerative responses to angry faces in healthy young women. Behav. Neurosci. 115, 238–242
  • 44: van Honk, J. et al. (2005) Testosterone reduces unconscious fear but not consciously experienced anxiety: implications for the disorders of fear and anxiety. Biol. Psychiatry 58, 218–225
  • 48: Hermans, E.J. et al. (2006) Testosterone administration reduces empathetic behavior: a facial mimicry study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31, 859–866
  • 49: Van Honk, J. et al. (2011) Testosterone administration impairs cognitive empathy in women depending on second-to-fourth digit ratio. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1011891108
  • 51: Bos, P.A. et al. (2010) Testosterone decreases trust in socially naive humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 9991–9995
  • 55: Bos, P.A. et al. (2011) Acute effects of steroid hormones and neuropeptides on human social-emotional behavior: a review of single administration studies. Front. Neuroendocrinol. DOI: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2011.01.002
  • 56: Archer, J. (2006) Testosterone and human aggression: an evaluation of the challenge hypothesis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, 319–345
  • 57: Zak, P.J. et al. (2009) Testosterone administration decreases generosity in the ultimatum game. PLoS ONE 4, e8330
  • 58: Ronay, R. and Galinsky, A.D. (2011) Lex talionis: testosterone and the law of retaliation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 47, 702–705
  • 63: Derntl, B. et al. (2009) Amygdala activity to fear and anger in healthy young males is associated with testosterone. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 687–693
  • 64: Manuck, S.B. et al. (2010) Salivary testosterone and a trinucleotide (CAG) length polymorphism in the androgen receptor gene predict amygdala reactivity in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 94–104
  • 65: Stanton, S.J. et al. (2009) Endogenous testosterone levels are associated with amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex responses to anger faces in men but not women. Biol. Psychol. 81, 118–122
  • 66: Hermans, E.J. et al. (2008) Exogenous testosterone enhances responsiveness to social threat in the neural circuitry of social aggression in humans. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 263–270
  • 67: van Wingen, G. et al. (2010) Testosterone reduces amygdala-orbitofrontal cortex coupling. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 105–113
  • 68: Volman, I. et al. (2011) Endogenous testosterone modulates prefrontal-amygdala connectivity during social emotional behavior. Cereb. Cortex DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhr001
  • 79: Nyby, J.G. (2008) Reflexive testosterone release: a model system for studying the nongenomic effects of testosterone upon male behavior. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 29, 199–210
  • 80: Bhasin, S. et al. (2006) Testosterone therapy in adult men with androgen deficiency syndromes: an endocrine society clinical practice guideline. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 91, 1995–2010
  • 81: Aarts, H. and van Honk, J. (2009) Testosterone and unconscious positive priming increase human motivation separately. Neuroreport 20, 1300–1303
  • 82: Hermans, E.J. et al. (2010) Effects of exogenous testosterone on the ventral striatal BOLD response during reward anticipation in healthy women. Neuroimage 52, 277–283
  • 89: Hermans, E.J. et al. (2006) A single administration of testosterone reduces fear-potentiated startle in humans. Biol. Psychiatry 59, 872–874
  • 90: Viau, V. (2002) Functional cross-talk between the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal and -adrenal axes. J. Neuroendocrinol. 14, 506–513
  • 91: Hermans, E.J. et al. (2007) Exogenous testosterone attenuates the integrated central stress response in healthy young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32, 1052–1061
Postado (editado)

Existem vários foruns de discussão sobre esse assunto de ser Alpha e que liberam materiais para download até mesmo internacionais e eles traduzem, os principais que conheço é o www.PUAbase.com e o www.puas.club são boas comunidades para desenvolvimento pessoal.

Editado por LuckSoah

Crie uma conta ou entre para comentar

Você precisar ser um membro para fazer um comentário

Criar uma conta

Crie uma nova conta em nossa comunidade. É fácil!

Crie uma nova conta

Entrar

Já tem uma conta? Faça o login.

Entrar Agora
×
×
  • Criar Novo...